Wednesday, April 14, 2010
Reinforcement
Reinforcement is distinct from reproductive character displacement. This term was coined by Butlin to describe situations in which taxa come into secondary contact and produce hybrids with are completely unfit. In this case gene flow is not possible, as the hybrid will not be able to produce surviving offspring. This means the two populations are already good species, and so any further enhancement of prezygotic isolation would have nothing to do with speciation and should not be considered reinforcement. In contrast, in the case of reinforcement the hybrids are only partially unfit, so gene flow is possible, and the speciation process is not complete. Enhancement of prezygotic isolation in this case is relevant to speciation.
As Felsentein (1981) explained, it should be theoretically difficult for reinforcement to evolve. Reinforcement requires that individuals assortatively mate with others that share similar phenotypic traits. This means that there must be strong linkage disequilibrium between alleles underlying the characters that have undergone divergent evolution between the two populations, and the alleles underlying mating preferences. Felsenstein pointed out that although this linkage may be generated by selection, recombination often destroys it. Unless the two loci are tightly linked, it should be difficult to overcome recombination.
One way to look for evidence of reinforcement is to quantify the level of prezygotic isolation between populations in sympatry and in allopatry. The prediction would be that prezygotic isolation would be higher between the populations in sympatry. This is because there would only by hybridization between the two populations they come into contact, and so in sympatry there would be an opportunity for natural selection to increase the strength of adaptations leading to prezygotic isolation, where in allopatry there would not.
Could you use this kind of a prediction to look for reinforcement in white sands lizards? Or is reinforcement even a relevant concept to apply to this system? Upon first examination I thought it would make sense that reinforcement could be going on at the ecotone. After all, you have white populations of lizards in white sands, dark populations in the various surrounding desert habitats, and intermediately colored lizards at the ecotone. So it seemed to me that these intermediately colored individuals could be hybrids of dark and light populations that had diverged in allopatry and come back into secondary contact. Turns out it is likely that this isn’t the case. For one thing, there really aren’t any light populations and dark populations located close enough together to suggest that frequent hybridization of parental populations at the ecotone is common. In addition, the genetic basis for dark and blanched coloration in Aspidoscelis and Sceloporus is a simple dominant recessive relationship. In Aspidoscelis the mutation in Mc1r causing blanched coloration is recessive, while in Sceloporus it is dominant (Rosenblum et al 2009). So for example, if you had a mating between a dark (heterozygous or homozygous dominant) Aspidoscelis and light (homozygous recessive) Aspidoscelis, you might get either a light colored hybrid or a dark colored hybrid, but you shouldn’t see an intermediately colored individual. So, though it is interesting to contemplate where these intermediately colored individuals are coming from, their existence may not be relevant in the case of our discussion of reinforcement. It seems from the evidence we have currently that reinforcement of prezygotic isolation is not an important mechanism of speciation in the white sands system.
Tuesday, March 9, 2010
Behavioral Isolation
This week we read chapter 6 or Speciation, titled “Behavioral and Nonecological Isolation.” Coyne and Orr describe 4 main categories of “evolutionary forces” that lead to behavioral isolation. These include 1) selection on mate preference, 2) selection on certain traits, 3) genetic drift, and 4) nongenetic mechanisms.
1) In the first category, initial selection pressures act on mate preferences of individuals either directly or indirectly. Direct selection on mate preferences increases the immediate fitness of the chooser by improving the chooser’s ability to acquire resources, or allowing the chooser to avoid deleterious features associated with mating. Indirect selection on preference includes selection that does not increase the chooser’s immediate fitness. For example, in the runaway model of selection preference for a certain trait is genetically correlated with the gene conferring that trait.
2) In the second category there is selection on traits that improve the attractiveness of the bearer to the opposite sex, improve the ability of the bearer to overcome competition from members of the same sex, or that facilitate species recognition. This last category includes trait changes that evolve through natural selection with preferences coevolving. It also includes the evolution of reinforcement, which improves the ability of individuals to discriminate conspecifics from heterospecifics in order to reduce maladaptive hybridization.
3) Genetic drift may affect behavioral isolation if nonselective changes in allele frequencies affect a signal or a preference.
4) Nongenetic mechanisms of isolation include cultural drift, and host parasitism.
It seems to me that direct selection on traits would be most important for behavioral isolation of populations of white sands lizards and dark soils lizards, especially through species recognition. The two subcategories for the species recognition section seem quite similar to me. In the first scenario, natural selection causes a trait to change, and preferences for that trait coevolve because mating with individuals with that trait will improve the fitness of offspring. In the second scenario, evolution favors discrimination between individuals with different traits to reduce the chance of producing less fit offspring. It almost seems like two sides of the same coin; recognize a trait and mate with that individual to create more fit offspring, or recognize a trait and don’t mate with that individual to avoid making less fit offspring.
Coyne and Orr distinguish the two by emphasizing that in the first scenario there need not be a closely related sympatric species present. Divergent selection in different habitats could cause changes in traits and preferences for those traits, which could lead to behavioral isolation between locally adapted populations. The book cites the example of divergent selection on body size for resource acquisition in benthic and limnetic morphs of sticklebacks .
In the case of reinforcement, there must be a related sympatric species present, and the fact that hybrids are less fit leads to selection favoring the avoidance of hybridization through increased ability to recognize conspecifics. So I guess if the difference between these two ideas is just whether there is divergence of locally adapted populations or of sympatric sister taxa, then the lizards in white sands would fall under the first scenario. This distinction is still a little fuzzy to me so if anyone has any comments please feel free to let me know where I’m going wrong.
Wednesday, March 3, 2010
Ecological Isolation
Thursday, February 25, 2010
The "Species" of White Sands
Sunday, February 21, 2010
Speciation models
Parapatric speciation refers to the divergence of populations between which there is some gene flow. One model of parapatric speciation is the clinal model. In this model a species is distributed continuously across a variable environment. Subpopulations adapt to local habitats, but adaptation is hindered by gene flow from nearby ecologically distinct populations. When populations have become differentiated enough, reinforcement may play a role in generating reproductive isolation. In the stepping stone model of parapatric speciation, there are discrete populations with restricted gene exchange. Since populations are discrete it is easier for selection or drift to result in reproductive isolation than in the clinal model.
Though white sands and dark soils lizards have not speciated, we can still think of which of these models is most applicable to divergence of these populations. The first question we should answer is how much gene flow is occuring between white sands and dark soils lizards. Previous research has shown that there is some gene flow, though the amount differs between the different species of lizards (Rosenblum 2006). This means that the parapatric models are more applicable than the allopatric models. Next question: are populations distributed continuously over a variable environment, or are there discrete populations? While the environment is definitely variable from white sands to dark soils, the amount of connectivity between populations is also different between different species of lizards. For example, Holbrookia maculata show the most genetic differentiation between populations, and also the largest color difference between white sands and dark soils populations. Sceloporus undulatus shows an intermediate level of phenotypic and genetic difference between populations, and Aspidoscelis inornata shows the least of both (Rosenblum 2006).
This evidence shows that Holbrookia populations are probably the most discrete, with Sceloporus populations occurring in a less discrete distribution, and Aspidoscelis distributed almost continuously. So maybe the stepping stone model is most likely for Holbrookia, clinal for Aspidoscelis, and some combination for Sceloporus? And if the models are different, does this mean that different types of reproductive isolation could be important for different species? Reinforcement is thought to be important for the clinal model, but in the stepping stone model premating isolation might evolve more easily as a byproduct of selection or drift. Hmmmmmm…
Monday, February 15, 2010
Barriers leading to reproductive isolation
1. Premating isolation barriers- barriers that impede gene flow before transfer of gametes to members of other species.
a. Behavioral isolation: Members of different species do not initiate courtship or copulation with each other due to a lack of “cross-attraction”.
b. Ecological isolation: Barriers to reproduction are direct byproducts of adaptation to the local environment. This category includes isolation because of differences in habitat preference, timing of breeding, and pollinator interactions.
c. Mechanical isolation: Members of different species do not reproduce because they have incompatible reproductive structures.
d. Mating system isolation: This barrier refers to the evolution of partial or complete self fertilization or the asexual production of offspring, which can result in the formation of new species.
2. Postmating, prezygotic isolation barriers- barriers that act after transfer of gametes but before fertilization takes place.
a. Copulatory behavioral isolation: The behaviors of individuals during copulation do not allow fertilization to occur.
b. Gametic isolation: The transferred gametes cannot effectively cause fertilization.
3. Postzygotic isolation barriers- barriers resulting in hybrid sterility and inviability.
a. Extrinsic barriers: Sterility and inviability are results of the biotic or abiotic environment. This category includes ecological inviability, where hybrids have a lower fitness because of a failure to find an appropriate ecological niche. Behavioral sterility, where hybrids cannot obtain mates, is also included.
b. Intrinsic barriers: Sterility and inviability are results of developmental problems. Hybrid inviability is a result of developmental problems causing full or partial lethality. Hybrid sterility may be physiological or behavioral; physiological sterility leads to problems in the development of gametes or reproductive organs, while behavioral leads to developmental problems causing hybrids to be incapable of successful courtship.
Which of these potential barriers could be acting currently among populations of lizards in white sands and dark soils? Of the premating isolation category, behavioral and ecological isolation both seem possible. In fact, it seems like a combination of these two types would be most likely. As discussed previously, behavioral isolation could be a result of either a preference for local mates that are phenotypically similar, or a lack of recognition of mates from populations adapted to living in different environments. Ecological isolation is certainly important in terms of habitat preference; if white lizards and dark lizards don’t encounter each other because they live in different areas, this will result in reproductive isolation. I think ecological isolation would be important over most of the habitat, while behavioral isolation may be important at the ecotone.
In terms of postzygotic isolation, extrinsic barriers seem most relevant to the white sands system. Depending on the genetic basis of dorsal coloration (which is different among different lizard species in white sands) hybrids could suffer from ecological inviability. The mutation that causes blanched coloration is dominant in one white sands species and recessive in another. Either way, if a hybrid finds itself not well matched to its substrate and can’t effectively choose a better niche, it won’t do as well (it will probably end up as food for someone else).
From reading about all these different isolation barriers I have come to the realization that as I continue on with my plans to investigate behavioral premating reproductive isolation in these lizards, I can’t overlook the possibility that other reproductive barriers could be playing an important role as well, and I need to be aware that many factors have likely shaped the system as it is today.
Tuesday, February 9, 2010
Genetic lines of least resistance, continued
I just wanted to add a few comments on the subject. Schluter mentions that when traits covary (in the ways Simone described earlier) the amount that one trait covaries with another will affect how far evolution will be pulled away from the direction in which selection is at its strongest. Take the finch beak example that Simone gave, and imagine that selection is strongest in the direction favoring an extremely long, narrow beak. If beak width and length depend on each other to a great extent, so that selection on one automatically changes the other dramatically (selection for a longer beak leads to a wider beak too), then this interaction would lead to evolution in a direction that is greatly skewed off of the path of strongest directional selection. If the traits did not affect each other as much, the direction of evolution would deviate from the direction of strongest selection to a smaller extent.
Schluter mentions that over time this effect of genetic constraint can be overcome and a population may make it to the top of an adaptive peak. But the stronger the covariance between the traits is, the stronger the skew will be away from the direction of strongest selection, and the longer it will take to make it up the peak.
One cool result of this whole covariance thing is that the degree of covariance between traits can help you more accurately predict the direction of evolution. If you realize that selection is strongest in a certain direction on an adaptive landscape, you can examine phenotypic traits that would be affected by selection, and based on how these traits covary you could make predictions about the direction of evolution along the landscape. Neat!
Now that’s really it for the Ecology of Adaptive Radiation…Speciation here we come!
Monday, February 8, 2010
Divergence Along Genetic Lines of Least Resistance & Final Conclusions
Tuesday, February 2, 2010
Reproductive isolation and ecological speciation
There are two models of how divergent selection causes reproductive isolation and ecological speciation:
By-product speciation is the first model. In the case of by-product speciation, divergent natural selection on phenotypes causes reproductive isolation to evolve incidentally between populations in different environments. Reproductive isolation is considered a “by-product” because it is not directly favored by selection. Both postmating and premating isolation can occur through by-product speciation. Postmating isolation evolves when genetic changes favored in different populations in different environments are incompatible when recombined in hybrids (this is known as hybrid inviability). Premating isolation can occur if mating preferences are genetically correlated with morphological or physiological traits that are the targets of divergent selection.
The second model Schluter mentions is competitive speciation. When intermediate genotypes in a population are at a fitness disadvantage selection may favor a population split leading to reproductive isolation. Competitive speciation is considered to be reinforcement if a period of allopatry leads to incomplete pre- and postmating isolation, and then when the populations come back together hybrids are selected against because they are at a fitness disadvantage. The process is known as sympatric speciation when there is no allopatric phase, and speciation is initiated by disruptive selection in a population. These two versions of competitive speciation are distinct from the by-product model because reproductive isolation is directly favored by natural selection in the sympatric phase.
I am interested in investigating whether premating isolation has occurred in white sands and dark soils lizards. In the by-product model, premating isolation between populations can develop when populations are in different selection environments, and mate preferences are linked to traits that are affected by natural selection in these differing environments.
What would have to occur for premating isolation to evolve between white sands and dark soils populations? First of all, the populations would have to be diverging in different selection environments. Check. White sands populations differ dramatically from dark soils populations in skin color, among other things. Second of all, divergent natural selection would have to affect traits linked to mate preferences or recognition. Here’s where things get a little more ambiguous. All three lizard species in the area have social signaling patches that are used in inter- and intrasexual interactions. The colors of these patches are different between white sands and dark soils populations. We know that this difference in skin color is linked to differences in the Mc1r gene. Mutation of Mc1r in white sands lizards has led to adaptive blanched dorsal skin coloration, and this change has affected the coloration of the social signaling patches as well.
So yes, social signaling patch color, which is involved in social interactions, is diverging due to natural selection in different environments. But is social signaling patch color related to mate choice or recognition? Maybe mate choice is linked to chemical secretions or social interactions that are divergent between populations by chance, not because of divergent natural selection? Or, for that matter, is there even mate choice occurring in these lizards? Maybe they would just be game for mating with any conspecific they run into. These are questions I’m hoping to begin answering during my first field season this spring. Okay, this is getting long, so I’ll stop…but if anyone has any input on my ideas I would love to hear it!
Saturday, January 30, 2010
Ecological Opportunity and White Sands
Simone here.
Tuesday, January 26, 2010
Thanks Kayla- nice coverage of character displacement.
I'd like to add one thing to that too:
Schluter focuses a lot on character displacement between closely related taxa...
However, character displacement can occur between distantly related taxa as well, it just seems to be less studied. For example, certainly the presence of other competing lizards NOT closely related to Sceloporus undulatus in New Mexico will cause them to use a smaller portion of their available resources.
Consider S. undulatus in surrounding dark soils habitat; there are several other competing lizards (such as Uta stansburiana pictured above), that may reduce the available niche space for Sceloporus. There are several potentially competing lizard species in the dark soils surrounding White Sands... but in White Sands itself, there are only three lizard species. Does this then mean that Sceloporus (and other White Sands inhabitants) are able to use a greater proportion of their habitat than members of their ancestral populations in dark soils? In other words, is something... the opposite... of character displacement occurring in White Sands?
We'll talk more about ecological opportunity and ecological release next week.
And I'm burnt out so... perhaps a little more later.
Monday, January 25, 2010
Mechanisms of divergence and character displacement
Schluter states that there are three main patterns found in nature that indicate character displacement has occurred. The first is exaggerated divergence in sympatry, where species found together are more different phenotypically than in areas where they do not coexist. The second is trait over-dispersion. This occurs when the mean phenotypes found within a species are more evenly spaced along a trait axis than would be expected from a random sample. The final pattern, species-for-species matching, is defined as replicated guild structure between independently evolving sets of species. There are six criteria for assessing whether character displacement has occurred between different species:
1. Phenotypic differences between species should have a genetic basis.
2. Chance should be ruled out as an explanation of the pattern.
3. Differences must represent evolutionary shifts rather than species sorting.
4. Shifts in resource use should match changes in morphology.
5. Environmental differences between sympatric and allopatric sites must be controlled for.
6. Independent evidence should be gained that similar phenotypes compete for resources.
So what’s the relevance of character displacement to the lizards of white sands? Is character displacement likely to occur between these three species sometime in the future? For lizards in the white sands area there are fewer competitors, so the interspecific competition for resources may not be strong enough to lead to the evolution of character displacement. However, there are also fewer resources in white sands than in the rest of the desert, which should increase interspecific competiton. Looks like it’s time for some field work examining the overlap in the niches of the different species of white sands lizards!
Schluter also listed some other mechanisms besides competition that may promote divergence. The one that seemed most relevant to white sands is the process of the evolution of food webs. Through this mechanism divergence occurs in newly colonized areas where there were originally few inhabitants, and a new food web is essentially generated from scratch. When this happens many species and populations undergo trophic level transitions, where they begin to take advantage of resources from other trophic levels than they did previously. White sands has few inhabitants, and so colonists may diverge through the process of exploiting new resources and through the generation of a new food web.
Thanks for reading…take it away Simone!